Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorFrasnelli, E
dc.contributor.authorRobert, T
dc.contributor.authorChow, PKY
dc.contributor.authorScales, B
dc.contributor.authorGibson, S
dc.contributor.authorManning, N
dc.contributor.authorPhilippides, AO
dc.contributor.authorCollett, TS
dc.contributor.authorHempel De Ibarra, N
dc.date.accessioned2021-01-04T07:36:34Z
dc.date.issued2020-12-28
dc.description.abstractHoneybees [1] and bumblebees [2] perform learning flights on leaving a newly discovered flower. During these flights, bees spend a portion of the time turning back to face the flower when they can memorise views of the flower and its surroundings. In honeybees, learning flights become longer, when the reward offered by a flower is increased [3]. We show here that bumblebees behave in a similar way and we add that bumblebees face an artificial flower more when the concentration of the sucrose solution that the flower provides is higher. The surprising finding is that a bee’s size determines what a bumblebee regards as a 'low' or a 'high' concentration and so affects its learning behaviour. The larger bees in a sample of foragers only enhance their flower facing when the sucrose concentration is in the upper range of the flowers that are naturally available to bees [4]. In contrast, smaller bees invest the same effort in facing flowers, whether the concentration is high or low, but their effort is less than that of larger bees. The way in which different sized bees distribute their effort when learning about flowers parallels the foraging behaviour of a colony. Large bumblebees [5, 6] are able to carry larger loads and explore further from the nest than smaller ones [7]. Small ones with a smaller flight range and carrying capacity cannot afford to be as selective and so accept a wider range of flowers.en_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipLeverhulme Trusten_GB
dc.identifier.citationPublished online 28 December 2020
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.cub.2020.11.062
dc.identifier.grantnumberUF130569en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberRPG-2012-677en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberEM-2016-066en_GB
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10871/124274
dc.language.isoenen_GB
dc.publisherElsevier (Cell Press)en_GB
dc.relation.urlhttps://doi.org/10.24378/exe.2864en_GB
dc.rights.embargoreasonUnder embargo until 28 December 2021 in compliance with publisher policyen_GB
dc.rights© 2020. This version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/  
dc.titleSmall and large bumblebees invest differently when learning about flowers (article)en_GB
dc.typeArticleen_GB
dc.date.available2021-01-04T07:36:34Z
dc.identifier.issn0960-9822
dc.descriptionThis is the author accepted manuscript. The final version is available from Elsevier via the DOI in this articleen_GB
dc.descriptionThe dataset associated with this article is available in ORE at https://doi.org/10.24378/exe.2864en_GB
dc.identifier.journalCurrent Biologyen_GB
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/  en_GB
dcterms.dateAccepted2020-11-24
exeter.funder::Leverhulme Trusten_GB
rioxxterms.versionAMen_GB
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2020-11-24
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_GB
refterms.dateFCD2020-12-28T20:39:03Z
refterms.versionFCDAM
refterms.panelAen_GB


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

© 2020. This version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/  
Except where otherwise noted, this item's licence is described as © 2020. This version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/