Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorGordon, DS
dc.contributor.authorLea, SEG
dc.date.accessioned2017-03-07T14:13:10Z
dc.date.issued2016-07-08
dc.description.abstract© The Author(s) 2016.''Moralistic'' punishment of free riders can provide a beneficial reputation, but the immediate behavior is costly to the punisher. In Study 1, we investigated whether variation in status would be perceived to offset or mitigate the costs of punishment. One hundred and nineteen participants were presented with a vignette describing a punishment scenario. Participants predicted whether punishment would occur, how successful it would be, and indicated their attitude to the punisher. Participants believed only intervention by a high-status (HS) individual would be successful and that low-status (LS) individuals would not intervene at all. HS individuals predicted to punish successfully were seen as more formidable and likable. Study 2 investigated whether punishment was necessary to maintain an HS position. One hundred and seventeen participants were presented with a vignette describing a punishment scenario. Participants were asked to indicate whether they wished to be led by the punisher. HS individuals who did not punish were less likely to be chosen as leaders compared to HS punishers, whereas LS individuals who punished were no more or less likely to be chosen than nonpunishers. The results of both studies suggest that only HS individuals are expected to punish, likely because such a position offsets some of the costs of punishment. As a result, only HS individual can access the reputation benefits from punishment. Furthermore, an HS position may be dependent on the willingness to punish antisocial behavior. The ramifications that these results may have for the evolution of moralistic punishment are discussed.en_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipThe author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: We would like to acknowledge the financial support provided for this project by the College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter.en_GB
dc.identifier.citationVol. 14, Iss. 3, pp. 1 - 14en_GB
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/1474704916658042
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10871/26300
dc.language.isoenen_GB
dc.publisherSAGE Publicationsen_GB
dc.rights(c) The Author(s) 2016. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 3.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).en_GB
dc.subjectpunishmenten_GB
dc.subjectstatusen_GB
dc.subjectfairnessen_GB
dc.subjectthird-partyen_GB
dc.subjectleadershipen_GB
dc.subjectreputationen_GB
dc.titleWho punishes? The status of the punishers affects the perceived success of, and indirect benefits from, ''moralistic'' punishmenten_GB
dc.typeArticleen_GB
dc.date.available2017-03-07T14:13:10Z
dc.descriptionPublisheden_GB
dc.descriptionJournal Articleen_GB
dc.descriptionThis is the final version of the article. Available from SAGE Publications via the DOI in this record.en_GB
dc.identifier.eissn1474-7049
dc.identifier.journalEvolutionary Psychologyen_GB


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record