Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorGoumas, M
dc.contributor.authorCollins, TR
dc.contributor.authorFordham, L
dc.contributor.authorKelley, LA
dc.contributor.authorBoogert, NJ
dc.date.accessioned2020-07-09T15:04:54Z
dc.date.issued2020-09-04
dc.description.abstractWith an increasing human population and expansion of urban settlements, wild animals are often exposed to humans. As humans may be a threat, a neutral presence, or a source of food, animals will benefit from continuously assessing the potential risk posed by humans in order to respond appropriately. Herring gulls (Larus argentatus) are increasingly breeding and foraging in urban areas, and thus have many opportunities to interact with humans. We recently found that herring gulls take longer to approach food when being watched by a human. However, it is not known whether aversion to human gaze arises from experience with humans, and whether individual differences in responsiveness are a result of differential exposure. Here, we test whether herring gulls’ responses to human gaze differ according to their age class and urbanisation of their habitat. We measured the gulls’ flight initiation distance when an experimenter approached with either a direct or averted gaze. Neither gull age class nor urbanisation significantly influenced the effect of human gaze on flight initiation distance. However, as recently fledged juveniles responded strongly to the experimenter’s gaze, aversion to human gaze may not require extensive exposure to humans to develop. Gulls in urban areas could be approached more closely than those in rural areas, consistent with findings in other species. These results indicate that gaze aversion is present early in development and that exposure to humans may influence gulls’ responses to perceived risk from humans. Investigating the processes generating individual differences in responses to humans will provide further insights into human-wildlife interactions and the effects of urbanisation.en_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipRoyal Societyen_GB
dc.identifier.citationVol. 168, pp. 83-88en_GB
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.08.008
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10871/121871
dc.language.isoenen_GB
dc.publisherElsevieren_GB
dc.rights.embargoreasonUnder embargo until 4 September 2021 in compliance with publisher policyen_GB
dc.rights© 2020. This version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/  en_GB
dc.subjectHerring gullen_GB
dc.subjectflight initiation distanceen_GB
dc.subjectgaze aversionen_GB
dc.subjectrisk perceptionen_GB
dc.subjecturbanisationen_GB
dc.subjecturban-rural gradienten_GB
dc.titleHerring gull aversion to gaze in urban and rural human settlementsen_GB
dc.typeArticleen_GB
dc.date.available2020-07-09T15:04:54Z
dc.identifier.issn0003-3472
dc.descriptionThis is the author accepted manuscript. The final version is available from Elsevier via the DOI in this recorden_GB
dc.identifier.journalAnimal Behaviouren_GB
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/  en_GB
dcterms.dateAccepted2020-07-08
exeter.funder::Royal Society (Government)en_GB
rioxxterms.versionAMen_GB
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2020-07-08
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_GB
refterms.dateFCD2020-07-09T14:33:16Z
refterms.versionFCDAM
refterms.panelAen_GB


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

© 2020. This version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/  
Except where otherwise noted, this item's licence is described as © 2020. This version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/