Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorField, JP
dc.contributor.authorAccleton, C
dc.contributor.authorFoster, W
dc.date.accessioned2018-12-04T15:38:56Z
dc.date.issued2018-09-27
dc.description.abstractOrganisms can often benefit by distinguishing between different classes of individuals. An example is kin recognition, whereby individuals preferentially associate with or aid genetic relatives that bear matching recognition cues but reject others. Despite its potential benefits, however, kin recognition using genetically based cues is often weak or absent [1, 2, 3, 4]. A general explanation, termed “Crozier’s effect,” is that when individuals interact randomly, rarer cue alleles less often match cues of other individuals, and so are involved predominantly in “reject”-type interactions. If such interactions are more costly, positive frequency-dependent selection will erode the cue diversity upon which discrimination depends [4, 5]. Although widely cited [1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 8, 9], this idea lacks rigorous testing in the field. Here, we show how Crozier’s effect applies to interactions between hosts and conspecific parasites, and measure it using field data. In the wasp we studied, conspecific parasitism fits a key assumption of Crozier’s model: the same females act as both hosts and parasites. By exchanging offspring between nests experimentally, we find no evidence that females respond to genetically based cues associated with foreign offspring. Through measuring costs and benefits, however, we demonstrate a strong Crozier effect: because more parental investment is wasted when foreign offspring are rejected, interactions involving rejection have substantially lower payoffs than interactions involving acceptance. Costly rejection can thus eliminate cue diversity by causing selection against rare cue alleles, consistent with the absence of genetically based recognition that we observe. Females instead appear to rely on non-genetic cues that enable them to detect less than half of parasitic offspring.en_GB
dc.identifier.citationVol. 28 (20), pp. 3267 - 3272en_GB
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.cub.2018.08.014
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10871/34991
dc.language.isoenen_GB
dc.publisherElsevier (Cell Press)en_GB
dc.rights© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)en_GB
dc.subjectCrozier’s paradoxen_GB
dc.subjectkin recognitionen_GB
dc.subjectconspecific parasitismen_GB
dc.subjectcuckoo parasitismen_GB
dc.subjectbrood parasitismen_GB
dc.subjectparental careen_GB
dc.subjectwaspsen_GB
dc.subjectAmmophilaen_GB
dc.subjectrecognition allelesen_GB
dc.titleCrozier’s effect and the acceptance of intraspecific brood parasitesen_GB
dc.typeArticleen_GB
dc.date.available2018-12-04T15:38:56Z
dc.identifier.issn0960-9822
dc.descriptionThis is the final version. Available on open access from Elsevier via the DOI in this record. en_GB
dc.identifier.journalCurrent Biologyen_GB
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/en_GB
dcterms.dateAccepted2018-08-02
rioxxterms.versionVoRen_GB
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2018-09-27
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_GB
refterms.dateFCD2018-12-03T18:00:35Z
refterms.versionFCDP
refterms.dateFOA2018-12-04T15:38:59Z
refterms.panelAen_GB
refterms.depositExceptionpublishedGoldOA


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
Except where otherwise noted, this item's licence is described as © 2018 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)