dc.contributor.author | Guess, AM | |
dc.contributor.author | Nyhan, B | |
dc.contributor.author | Reifler, J | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2020-07-07T14:29:31Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2020-03-02 | |
dc.description.abstract | Although commentators frequently warn about echo chambers, little is known about the volume or slant of political misinformation that people consume online, the effects of social media and fact checking on exposure, or the effects of political misinformation on behaviour. Here, we evaluate these questions for websites that publish factually dubious content, which is often described as fake news. Survey and web-traffic data from the 2016 US presidential campaign show that supporters of Donald Trump were most likely to visit these websites, which often spread through Facebook. However, these websites made up a small share of people’s information diets on average and were largely consumed by a subset of Americans with strong preferences for pro-attitudinal information. These results suggest that the widespread speculation about the prevalence of exposure to untrustworthy websites has been overstated. | en_GB |
dc.description.sponsorship | European Union Horizon 2020 | en_GB |
dc.description.sponsorship | Poynter Institute | en_GB |
dc.description.sponsorship | Knight Foundation | en_GB |
dc.description.sponsorship | American Press Institute | en_GB |
dc.identifier.citation | Vol. 4, pp. 472 - 480 | en_GB |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1038/s41562-020-0833-x | |
dc.identifier.grantnumber | 682758 | en_GB |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10871/121820 | |
dc.language.iso | en | en_GB |
dc.publisher | Nature Research | en_GB |
dc.relation.url | https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/YLW1AZ | en_GB |
dc.rights.embargoreason | Under embargo until 2 September 2020 in compliance with publisher policy | en_GB |
dc.rights | © The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited 2020 | en_GB |
dc.title | Exposure to untrustworthy websites in the 2016 US election | en_GB |
dc.type | Article | en_GB |
dc.date.available | 2020-07-07T14:29:31Z | |
dc.description | This is the author accepted manuscript. The final version is available from Nature Research via the DOI in this record | en_GB |
dc.description | Data availability:
Data files necessary to replicate the results in this article are available at the following Dataverse repository: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/YLW1AZ | en_GB |
dc.description | Code availability:
R/Stata scripts that replicate the results in this article are available at the following Dataverse repository: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/YLW1AZ | en_GB |
dc.identifier.eissn | 2397-3374 | |
dc.identifier.journal | Nature Human Behaviour | en_GB |
dc.rights.uri | http://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserved | en_GB |
dcterms.dateAccepted | 2020-02-03 | |
exeter.funder | ::European Commission | en_GB |
rioxxterms.version | AM | en_GB |
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate | 2020-02-03 | |
rioxxterms.type | Journal Article/Review | en_GB |
refterms.dateFCD | 2020-07-07T14:26:19Z | |
refterms.versionFCD | AM | |
refterms.dateFOA | 2020-09-01T23:00:00Z | |
refterms.panel | C | en_GB |