Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorHaber, NA
dc.contributor.authorWieten, SE
dc.contributor.authorRohrer, JM
dc.contributor.authorArah, OA
dc.contributor.authorTennant, PWG
dc.contributor.authorStuart, EA
dc.contributor.authorMurray, EJ
dc.contributor.authorPilleron, S
dc.contributor.authorLam, ST
dc.contributor.authorRiederer, E
dc.contributor.authorHowcutt, SJ
dc.contributor.authorSimmons, AE
dc.contributor.authorLeyrat, C
dc.contributor.authorSchoenegger, P
dc.contributor.authorBooman, A
dc.contributor.authorDufour, M-SK
dc.contributor.authorO'Donoghue, AL
dc.contributor.authorBaglini, R
dc.contributor.authorDo, S
dc.contributor.authorTakashima, MDLR
dc.contributor.authorEvans, TR
dc.contributor.authorRodriguez-Molina, D
dc.contributor.authorAlsalti, TM
dc.contributor.authorDunleavy, DJ
dc.contributor.authorMeyerowitz-Katz, G
dc.contributor.authorAntonietti, A
dc.contributor.authorCalvache, JA
dc.contributor.authorKelson, MJ
dc.contributor.authorSalvia, MG
dc.contributor.authorParra, CO
dc.contributor.authorKhalatbari-Soltani, S
dc.contributor.authorMcLinden, T
dc.contributor.authorChatton, A
dc.contributor.authorSeiler, J
dc.contributor.authorSteriu, A
dc.contributor.authorAlshihayb, TS
dc.contributor.authorTwardowski, SE
dc.contributor.authorDabravolskaj, J
dc.contributor.authorAu, E
dc.contributor.authorHoopsick, RA
dc.contributor.authorSuresh, S
dc.contributor.authorJudd, N
dc.contributor.authorPeña, S
dc.contributor.authorAxfors, C
dc.contributor.authorKhan, P
dc.contributor.authorRivera Aguirre, AE
dc.contributor.authorOdo, NU
dc.contributor.authorSchmid, I
dc.contributor.authorFox, MP
dc.date.accessioned2022-11-04T10:02:03Z
dc.date.issued2022-08-04
dc.date.updated2022-11-03T19:16:02Z
dc.description.abstractWe estimated the degree to which language used in the high profile medical/public health/epidemiology literature implied causality using language linking exposures to outcomes and action recommendations; examined disconnects between language and recommendations; identified the most common linking phrases; and estimated how strongly linking phrases imply causality. We searched and screened for 1,170 articles from 18 high-profile journals (65 per journal) published from 2010-2019. Based on written framing and systematic guidance, three reviewers rated the degree of causality implied in abstracts and full text for exposure/outcome linking language and action recommendations. Reviewers rated the causal implication of exposure/outcome linking language as None (no causal implication) in 13.8%, Weak 34.2%, Moderate 33.2%, and Strong 18.7% of abstracts. The implied causality of action recommendations was higher than the implied causality of linking sentences for 44.5% or commensurate for 40.3% of articles. The most common linking word in abstracts was "associate" (45.7%). Reviewers' ratings of linking word roots were highly heterogeneous; over half of reviewers rated "association" as having at least some causal implication. This research undercuts the assumption that avoiding "causal" words leads to clarity of interpretation in medical research.en_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipEuropean Union Horizon 2020en_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipAustralian Research Council (ARC)en_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipNational Institute of Mental Healthen_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipNational Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineeringen_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipNational Center for Advancing Translational Sciences UCLA Clinical Translational Science Instituteen_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipBloomberg American Health Initiativeen_GB
dc.description.sponsorshipKaren Toffler Charity Trusten_GB
dc.identifier.citationPublished online 4 August 2022en_GB
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwac137
dc.identifier.grantnumber842817en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberCE170100005en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberT32MH122357en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberR01MH115487en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberR01EB027650en_GB
dc.identifier.grantnumberUL1TR001881en_GB
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10871/131621
dc.identifierORCID: 0000-0001-7744-3780 (Kelson, Mark J)
dc.language.isoenen_GB
dc.publisherOxford University Pressen_GB
dc.relation.urlhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35925053en_GB
dc.relation.urlhttps://osf.io/jtdaz/en_GB
dc.rights.embargoreasonUnder embargo until 4 August 2023 in compliance with publisher policy
dc.rights© The Author(s) 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)en_GB
dc.subjectassociationen_GB
dc.subjectcausal inferenceen_GB
dc.subjectcausal languageen_GB
dc.subjectobservational studyen_GB
dc.titleCausal and associational language in observational health research: A systematic evaluationen_GB
dc.typeArticleen_GB
dc.date.available2022-11-04T10:02:03Z
dc.identifier.issn0002-9262
exeter.place-of-publicationUnited States
dc.descriptionThis is the author accepted manuscript. The final version is available from Oxford University Press via the DOI in this record. en_GB
dc.descriptionData, data analysis code, and materials are available on the Open Science Framework project https://osf.io/jtdaz/.en_GB
dc.identifier.eissn1476-6256
dc.identifier.journalAmerican Journal of Epidemiologyen_GB
dc.rights.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserveden_GB
dcterms.dateAccepted2022-07-26
rioxxterms.versionAMen_GB
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2022-08-04
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_GB
refterms.dateFCD2022-11-04T09:41:09Z
refterms.versionFCDAM
refterms.dateFOA2022-11-04T10:02:10Z
refterms.panelBen_GB
refterms.dateFirstOnline2022-08-04


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record